Solana, P., Escámez, O., Casasanto, D., Chica, A. B., & Santiago, J. (2024). No support for a causal role of primary motor cortex in construing meaning from language: An rTMS study. Neuropsychologia, 196, 108832.
|
|
Solana, P. & Santiago, J. (2023). Worse than expected: A z-curve reanalysis of motor cortex stimulation studies of embodied language comprehension. Psicologica, 44, e15661.
|
|
Solana, P. (2023). The embodiment and disembodiment of language. Nature Reviews Psychology, 2, 391.
|
Solana, P., and Santiago, J. (2022). Does the involvement of motor cortex in embodied language comprehension stand on solid ground? A p-curve analysis and test for excess significance of the TMS and tDCS evidence. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 141, 104834.
|
|
Rodríguez-Cuadrado, S., Ojedo, F., Vicente-Conesa, F., Romero-Rivas, C., Sampedro, M. A., & Santiago, J. (2022). Sign iconicity helps learning new words for abstract concepts in a foreign language. Second Language Research, 39, 873-898.
|
|
Callizo-Romero, C., Tutnjevic, S., Pandza, M., Ouellet, M., Kranjec, A., Ilic, S., Gu, Y., Göksun, T., Chahboun, S., Casasanto, D., & Santiago, J. (2022). Does time extend asymmetrically into the past and the future? A multitask cross-cultural study. Language and Cognition, 14, 275-302.
|
|
Santiago, J. (2022). ¿Cómo pensamos sobre el tiempo? (How do we think about time?) In: León, J.A., Domínguez, A., y Alonso, M. A. (Eds.). Neurocognición y Lenguaje. Editorial Médica Panamericana
Beracci, A., Santiago, J., & Fabbri, M. (2021). The categorical use of a continuous time representation. Psychological Research, 86, 1015-1028.
|
|
Vinson, D., Jones, M., Sidhu, D. M., Lau-Zhu, A., Santiago, J., & Vigliocco, G. (2021). Iconicity emerges and is maintained in spoken language. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 150, 2293-2308.
|
|
Callizo-Romero, C., Tutnjevic, S., Pandza, M., Ouellet, M., Kranjec, A., Ilic, S., Gu, Y., Göksun, T., Chahboun, S., Casasanto, D., & Santiago, J. (2020). Temporal focus and time spatialization across cultures. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 27, 1247-1258.
|
|
Colling, L., Szucs, D., De Marco, D., Cipora, K., Ulrich, R., Nuerk, H-C., Soltanlou, M., Bryce, D., Chen, S-C., Schroeder, P. A., Henare, D. T., Chrystall, C. K., Corballis, P. M., Ansari, D., Goffin, C., Sokolowski, H. M., Hancock, P. J. B., Millen, A. E., Langton, S. R. H., Holmes, K. J., Saviano, M. S., Tummino, T. A., Lindemann, O., Zwaan, R. A., Lukavský, J., Becková, A., Vranka, M. A., Cutini, S., Mammarella, I. C., Mulatti, C., Bell, R., Buchner, A., Mieth, L., Röer, J. P., Klein, E., Huber, S., Moeller, K., Ocampo, B., Lupiáñez, J., Ortiz-Tudela, J., De la Fuente, J., Santiago, J., Ouellet, M., Hubbard, E. M., Toomarian, E. Y., Job, R., Treccani, B., & McShane, B. B. (2020). A multilab registered replication of the attentional SNARC effect. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 3, 143-162.
|
|
Spatola, N., Santiago, J., Beffara, B., Mermillod, M., Ferrand, L., & Ouellet, M. (2018). When the sad past is left: The mental metaphors between time, valence, and space. Frontiers in Psychology, 9:1019. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01019
|
|
Román, A., Flumini, A., & Santiago, J. (2018). Scanning of speechless comics changes spatial biases in mental model construction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. Biological Sciences, 373, 20170130.
|
|
Tamariz, M., Roberts, S., Martínez, I., & Santiago, J. (2018). The interactive origin of iconicity. Cognitive Science, 42, 334-349.
|
|
Aguirre, R., & Santiago, J. (2017). Do potential past and future events activate the Left-Right Mental Timeline? Psicológica, 38, 231-255.
|
|
Chahboun, S., Flumini, A., Pérez González, C., McManus, I. C., & Santiago, J. (2017). Reading and writing direction effects on the aesthetic appreciation of photographs. Laterality, 22, 313-339. [Free e-prints]
|
|
de la Fuente, J., Casasanto, D., Martínez-Cascales, J. I., and Santiago, J. (2017). Motor imagery shapes abstract concepts. Cognitive Science, 41, 1350-1360.
|
|
Flumini, A., y Santiago, J. (2016). Metáforas y conceptos abstractos: Las contribuciones del Grounded Cognition Lab de la Universidad de Granada. In: M. C. Horno Chéliz, I. Ibarretxe Antuñano, & J. L. Mendívil Giró (Eds.), Panorama actual de la ciencia del lenguaje. Primer sexenio de Zaragoza Lingüística. Zaragoza: Prensas Universitarias de Zaragoza (PUZ).
|
|
Damjanovic, L., & Santiago, J. (2016). Contrasting vertical and horizontal representations of affect in emotional visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23, 62-73.
|
|
Román, A., Flumini, A., Escobar, M., & Santiago, J. (2015). Reading and writing direction causes spatial biases in mental model construction in language understanding. Scientific Reports, 5:18248. doi:10.1038/srep18248
|
|
Pagán Cánovas, C., Valenzuela, J., & Santiago, J. (2015). Like the machete the snake: Integration of topic and vehicle in poetry comprehension reveals meaning construction processes. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 9, 385-393.
|
|
Aguirre, R., & Santiago, J. (2015). Do potential past and future events activate the Lateral Mental Timeline? In: Noelle, D. C., Dale, R., Warlaumont, A. S., Yoshimi, J., Matlock, T., Jennings, C. D., & Maglio, P. P. (Eds.). Proceedings of the 37th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society. (pp. 48-53). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
|
|
Chahboun, S., Flumini, A., Pérez González, C., McManus, I. C., & Santiago, J. (2015). Reading and writing direction effects on the aesthetic perception of photographs. In: Noelle, D. C., Dale, R., Warlaumont, A. S., Yoshimi, J., Matlock, T., Jennings, C. D., & Maglio, P. P. (Eds.). Proceedings of the 37th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society. (pp. 2242-2247). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
|
|
Alvarez de Cienfuegos, E, & Santiago, J. (2015, June 28). Sound symbolism and construal level. Pre-registered replication of Maglio et al (2014), JEP:GEN, 143, 1082-96, Study 3. Retrieved from osf.io/c6vhf
De la Fuente, J., Casasanto, D., & Santiago, J. (2015). Observed actions affect body-specific associations between space and valence. Acta Psychologica, 156, 32–36.
de la Fuente, J., Casasanto, D., Román, A., & Santiago, J. (2015). Can culture influence body-specific associations between space and valence? Cognitive Science, 39, 821-832.
|
|
Santiago, J., & Lakens, D. (2015). Can conceptual congruency effects between number, time, and space be accounted for by polarity correspondence? Acta Psychologica, 156, 179-191.
|
|
de la Fuente, J., Santiago, J., Román, A., Dumitrache, C., & Casasanto, D. (2014). When you think about it, your past is in front of you: How culture shapes spatial conceptions of time. Psychological Science, 25, 1682-1690.
|
|
de la Fuente, J., Casasanto, D., & Santiago, J. (2014). Observed motor actions affect valence judgments. In M. Bello P., Guarini M., McShane M. & Scassellati B. (Eds.) Proceedings of the 36th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 385-390). Austin TX: Cognitive Science Society.
|
|
Jones, M., Vinson, D., Clostre, N., Lau Zhu, A., Santiago, J., & Vigliocco, G. (2014). The Bouba Effect: Sound-shape iconicity in iterated and implicit learning. In M. Bello P., Guarini M., McShane M. & Scassellati B. (Eds.) Proceedings of the 36th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2459-2464). Austin TX: Cognitive Science Society.
|
|
Román, A., Flumini, A., Escobar, M., & Santiago, J. (2014). Reading and writing direction causes spatial biases in mental model construction during language understanding. In M. Bello P., Guarini M., McShane M. & Scassellati B. (Eds.) Proceedings of the 36th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2853-2858). Austin TX: Cognitive Science Society.
|
|
Santiago, J. (2014). Body Memory, Metaphor, and Movement. Sabine Koch, Thomas Fuchs, Michela Summa, & Cornelia Müller (Eds.). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins, 2012. vi + 468 pages, $149.00 (hardcover), ISBN 9789027213501. Metaphor and Symbol, 29(1), 62–65.
|
|
Martinez-Cascales, I., de la Fuente, J., Santiago, J. Sr., & Santiago, J. Jr. (2013). Space and time bisection in schizophrenia. Frontiers in Psychology, 4:823. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00823
|
|
Santiago, J., & Lakens, D. (2013). Polarity correspondence does not explain the SNARC effect. In M. Knauff, M. Pauen, N. Sebanz, & I. Wachmuz (Eds.), Proceedings of the 35th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1259–1264). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
|
|
Flumini, A., & Santiago, J. (2013). Time (also) flies from left to right... if it is needed! In M. Knauff, M. Pauen, N. Sebanz, & I. Wachmuz (Eds.), Proceedings of the 35th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2315–2320). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
|
|
Román, A., El Fathi, A., & Santiago, J. (2013). Spatial biases in understanding descriptions of static scenes: The role of reading and writing direction. Memory & Cognition, 41, 588-599.
|
|
Santiago, J., Ouellet, M., Román, A., & Valenzuela, J. (2012). Attentional factors in conceptual congruency. Cognitive Science, 36, 1051-1077.
|
|
Ouellet, M., Román, A., & Santiago, J. (2012). A multisensory interaction effect in the conceptual realm of time. Experimental Psychology, 59, 236-242.
|
|
Román, A., El Fathi, A., & Santiago, J. (2011). Spatial biases in understanding descriptions of static scenes: The role of reading and writing direction. In L. Carlson, C. Hoelscher, & T.F. Shipley (Eds.), Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 784-788). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
|
|
de la Fuente, J., Casasanto, D., Román, A., & Santiago, J. (2011). Searching for cultural influences on the body-specific association of preferred hand and emotional valence. In: L. Carlson, C. Hoelscher, & T.F. Shipley (Eds.), Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2616-2620). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
|
|
Santiago, J., Román, A. & Ouellet, M. (2011) Flexible foundations of abstract thought: A review and a theory. In: Maas, A. & Schubert, T. (Eds). Spatial Dimensions of Social Thought. Mouton de Gruyter.
|
|
Ouellet, M., Santiago, J., Israeli, Z., & Gabay, S. (2010). Is the future the right time? Experimental Psychology, 57, 308-314.
|
|
Ouellet, M., Santiago, J., Funes, M. J. & Lupiáñez, J. (2010). Thinking about the future moves attention to the right. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human, Perception and Performance, 36, 17-24.
Santiago, J., Román, A., Ouellet, M., Rodríguez, N., & Pérez-Azor, P. (2010). In hindsight, life flows from left to right. Psychological Research, 74(1), 59–70.
|
|
Coll-Florit, M., I. Castellón, S. Climent, & J. Santiago (2009). Realidad psicológica del aspecto léxico. Evidencias experimentales (Psychological reality of lexical aspect: Experimental evidence). In: Valenzuela, J., A. Rojo & C. Soriano (Eds.), Trends in Cognitive Linguistics: Theoretical and Applied Models. Ed. Peter Lang.
|
|
Ouellet, M., Santiago, J., Israeli, Z. & Gabay, S. (2009) Multimodal influences of orthographic directionality on the "Time is Space" conceptual metaphor. In N. A. Taatgen & H. van Rijn (Eds.), Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1840-1845). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
|
|
Santiago, J., Lupiáñez, J., Pérez, E., & Funes, M. J. (2007). Time (also) flies from left to right. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 512-516.
|
|
Torralbo, A., Santiago, J. & Lupiáñez, J. (2006). Flexible conceptual projection of time onto spatial frames of reference. Cognitive Science, 30, 745-757.
|
|